Why revelations have occurred on mountains?: Linking mystical experiences and cognitive neuroscience

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.044Get rights and content

Summary

The fundamental revelations to the founders of the three monotheistic religions, among many other revelation experiences, had occurred on a mountain. These three revelation experiences share many phenomenological components like feeling and hearing a presence, seeing a figure, seeing lights, and feeling of fear. In addition, similar experiences have been reported by non-mystic contemporary mountaineers. The similarities between these revelations on mountains and their appearance in contemporary mountaineers suggest that exposure to altitude might affect functional and neural mechanisms, thus facilitating the experience of a revelation. Different functions relying on brain areas such as the temporo-parietal junction and the prefrontal cortex have been suggested to be altered in altitude. Moreover, acute and chronic hypoxia significantly affect the temporo-parietal junction and the prefrontal cortex and both areas have also been linked to altered own body perceptions and mystical experiences. Prolonged stay at high altitudes, especially in social deprivation, may also lead to prefrontal lobe dysfunctions such as low resistance to stress and loss of inhibition. Based on these phenomenological, functional, and neural findings we suggest that exposure to altitudes might contribute to the induction of revelation experiences and might further our understanding of the mountain metaphor in religion.

Mystical and religious experiences are important not only to the mystic himself, but also to many followers, as it was indeed with respect to the leaders of the three monotheistic religions. Yet, concerning its subjective character, mystical experiences are almost never accessible to the scholars interested in examining them. The tools of cognitive neuroscience make it possible to approach religious and mystical experiences not only by the semantical analysis of texts, but also by approaching similar experiences in healthy subjects during prolonged stays at high altitude and/or in cognitive paradigms. Cognitive neurosciences, in turn, might profit from the research of mysticism in their endeavor to further our understanding of mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self consciousness.

Introduction

Mountains play an important role in the symbolic geography of traditions. Thus, in Hindu and Buddhism mount Meru serves as the axis mundi; in China and Judaism mountains Scham and Zion, respectively, are described as the dwelling place of the divine; upon Japanese and native American traditions mountains are also charged with divine power [1], [2]. Moreover, mountains often serve as a place of revelation. In fact, the revelations to the founders of the three western monotheistic religions – Moses, Jesus, and Mohammad – occurred on mountains (see Appendix). On mount Sinai Moses experienced his first revelation in the burning bush, and encountered the Hebrew God three more times (Exodus, 24: 10). Jesus was transfigured “up a high mountain apart” – identified as mount Tabor [3] or mount Hermon [2] – and appeared to Peter, John and James in a cloud of glory (Matthew 17: 1–8; Mark 9: 2–8; Luke 9: 28–36). In Islamic tradition, Prophet Mohammad had received the Qur’an while in solitude on mount Hira by a revelation of the archangel Gabriel (Al-Bukhari, 3) (see Table 1).

A mystical trend that invented a technique to induce revelation is the Hesychasm movement,1 a monastic practice of prayer in the oriental church who trained many western mystics during the 5th to the 15th centuries [3], [4], [5]. This technique uses physical maneuvers such as breath patterns, head movements and repetitions on specific prayers, and was supposed to prepare the mystic to the experience of revelation. In addition, the Hesychasm movement had most of its centers on high altitude mountains – first at mount Sinai and then at mount Athos. Others have suggested that this might have contributed to the experience of revelation [3], [4], [5], [6].

Modern reports on revelation-like experiences (i.e. experiences that include similar components to those described in revelations-experiences as below, without having a religious/mystical content) in high altitude mountaineers support the link between mountains and revelation. Thus, mountaineers have described the experience of feeling of a presence [7], [8], [9], [10], hearing of a presence [9], [10], complex visual hallucinations [9], body photism [9], autoscopic phenomena [9], [10], [11], vestibular manifestations [9], [12] and emotional manifestations, mainly fear [9], [10]. The later two frequently accompany autoscopic phenomena [13], [14], [15], [16]. In addition, these experiences appear especially in solitude [9], [10]. Stressing this latter link, the revelation experiences of Moses, Jesus and Mohammad also manifest similar experiences. With regard to complex visual hallucinations, Moses saw an angel in the burning bush (Exodus, 3: 2), Peter, James and John saw the transfigured Jesus and the images of Elias and Moses (Matthew, 17: 2–3). Mohammed encountered the angle Gabriel while on the mountain (Al-Bukhari, 3).2 All three experiences were accompanied by seeing of light or focal or diffuse increases in brightness:3 Moses saw the angel “in flames of fire” (Exodus, 3: 2), the transfiguration of Jesus is described as “his face shone like the sun, and his garments became white as light” (Matthew, 17: 2). The light in Mohammad’s revelation even gave mount Hira its name: “Jabal an-Nur” (“mountain of light”). With respect to auditory manifestations, Moses heard a voice speaking to him from the burning bush (Exodus, 3: 5–10); Jesus’ disciples heard a voice from a cloud, and Moses and Elias speaking to their master (Matthew, 17: 3,5); Mohammad heard the angel’s commandment to read (Al-Bukhari, 3). Fear is described in all three experiences (Exodus 3: 6; Matthew 17: 7–8; Al-Bukhari, 3) and Moses and Mohammad had their revelation after a long time of solitude on the mountain (Exodus, 3: 1; 34: 28; Al-Bukhari, 3).

The similarities between these revelations on mountains and their appearance in contemporary mountaineers suggest that exposure to altitude might affect brain functions, thus facilitating the experience of a revelation. Brugger et al. [9] proposed that stressful events, physically and emotionally while climbing mountains, cause release of endorphins, which are known to lower the threshold for temporal lobe epilepsy, which, in turn, may evoke revelation-like experiences such as hearing of sounds or voices, memory flashback, déjà vu, a sense that someone is nearby (feeling of a presence), autoscopic phenomena and emotional manifestations, most commonly fear [17]. Blanke et al. [18] suggested that feeling a presence, autoscopy, and hearing a presence might all relate to abnormal body processing or body schema. It has been proposed that such phenomena are due to interference with the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) and adjacent structures [16], [18], [19], [20]. Vestibular manifestations also accompany phenomena resulting from damage to this area [16], [20], [21] and multisensory integration in the temporo-parietal association areas at extreme altitude may be disturbed [10]. In addition, Brugger et al. [9] suggested that prolonged stay at high altitudes, especially in social deprivation, may also lead to prefrontal lobe dysfunctions such as low resistance to stress and loss of inhibition. Many of the latter dysfunctions are also frequently found during ecstatic experience [22]. Thus, although prolonged stay at extreme altitudes is associated with changes in many brain areas [23], [24], [25], the effects of acute and chronic hypoxia might preferentially affect two areas of the cerebral cortex, the TPJ and the prefrontal cortex, that have been linked to disturbed own body perceptions and mystical experiences.

Although the revelations discussed here had occurred in moderate altitude, it may be assumed that in subjects who are prone to mystical experiences, already moderate altitudes are sufficient to trigger revelation-like experiences and revelations. Accordingly, mountain-sickness has been reported in some individuals even in moderate altitudes [26], [27]. Interestingly, although autoscopic phenomena are experienced by jet aviators that are flying alone especially at high altitudes, helicopter pilots can also experience similar phenomena already at altitudes of 1500–3000 m [28], [29]. In fact, isolation in altitudes as well as sensory deprivation might add to pure altitude-related mechanisms, as the desert attracted various mystics throughout generations [30], [31]. Furthermore, mental imagery of heights might be associated with vestibular dysfunctions and anxiety [32]. These different findings might suggest that the frequent appearance of the mountain metaphor in revelation experiences of mystics [22] might be related to interference with functional and neural mechanisms of corporal awareness as well as other cortical functions at the TPJ and the prefrontal cortex. In addition, this suggests that mechanisms of corporal awareness and mystical experiences could scientifically be studied at high altitudes and compared to the same experiences and functions at lower levels of altitudes.

Mystical and religious experiences are important not only to the mystic himself, but also to many followers, as it was indeed with respect to the three religious leaders discussed here. Yet, concerning its subjective character, mystical experiences are almost never accessible to the scholars interested in examine them. Thus, some modern scholars of religious experience focused their inquiry on subjective experience or data, rather than theological data [33], [34], [35]. The neurocognitive approach we suggest here arms the mystical researcher with a new access to these experiences. The tools of cognitive neuroscience make it possible to approach religious and mystical experiences not only by semantical analysis of texts, but also by approaching similar experiences in healthy subjects4 in certain physical conditions such as prolonged stay in high altitude as well as by approaching similar experiences in neurological patients. Combined with analysis of performance in experimental paradigms, neuropsychological methods, lesion studies and neuroimaging, our understanding of the mountain metaphor in religion, as well as mystical experiences in general, might be advanced. In the current case, high altitudes seem to evoke quite similar experiences as reported by different mystical and religious figures throughout time. Thus, as wide as the mind of the mystic is enlarged, and might be different from the religious follower, the core of the experience might be shared and relates to similar functional and neural mechanisms, which are available to further scientific inquiry.

References (38)

  • W.O. Guldin et al.

    Is there a vestibular cortex?

    Trends Neurosci

    (1998)
  • M. Eliade

    Le mythe de l’eternel retour

    (1989)
  • D.L. Eck

    Mountains

  • F.X. Murphy

    Hesychasm

  • I. Hausherr

    La méthode d’Oraison hesychaste

    Orientalia Christiana

    (1929)
  • Hausherr I. Hésychasme et Prière. Orientalia Christiania Analecta 1966;176. Roma: Pont institutum orientalium...
  • G.A. Maloneys

    Mount athos

  • M. Herzog

    Annapurna

    (1952)
  • C. Clarke

    On surviving a bivouac at high altitude

    BMJ

    (1976)
  • P. Brugger et al.

    Hallucinatory experience in extreme-altitude climbers

    Neuropsychi Neuropsy Behav Neur

    (1999)
  • P.G. Firth et al.

    Transient high altitude neurological dysfunction: an origin in the temporoparietal cortex

    High Alt Med Biol

    (2004)
  • P. Habeler

    The lonely victory

    (1979)
  • T.H. Ravenhill

    Some experiences of mountain sickness in the andes

    Am J Trop Med Hyg

    (1913)
  • N. Lukianowicz

    Autoscopic phenomena

    Arch Neurol

    (1958)
  • O.J. Grüsser et al.

    The splitting of ‘I’ and ‘me’: Heautoscopy and related phenomena

  • T.R. Dening et al.

    Autoscopic phenomena

    Br J Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • O. Blanke et al.

    Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin

    Brain

    (2004)
  • D.L. Schomer et al.

    Temporolimbic epilepsy and behaviour

  • O. Blanke et al.

    Hearing of a presence

    Neurocase

    (2003)
  • Cited by (29)

    • Effect of hypoxia on human cognitive ability and indoor oxygen environment demand for sojourners at high altitude

      2021, Building and Environment
      Citation Excerpt :

      Regarding the thinking ability, Pu et al. found that a period of training at high altitude can slow down a soldier's mind, which deteriorated their coding ability and computing ability to levels below those measured before entering the plateau [38]. Arzy [39] suggested that continuous exposure to plateau or high-altitude (>5000 m) environments affects cognition, and others found that high-altitude exposure greatly influences the cognitive ability [40]. However, different opinions have been voiced as well.

    • Perceptual phenomena in destructured sensory fields: Probing the brain's intrinsic functional architectures

      2019, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      As a result, the supposed alterations in perceptual experience stemming from auditory entrainment remain tenuous until they are subject to more rigorous empirical evaluation. Destructured sensory fields can be reasonably implicated as antecedent factors in several well-documented cases of anomalous perception – for instance, the hallucinatory percepts reported by high-altitude mountaineers (Arzy et al., 2005; Brugger et al., 1999) and employees in highly protected-environments (Kellerman et al., 1977), as well as instances of so-called intensive care unit psychosis (Dyson, 1999). The autobiographical record of lone explorers in extreme situations is replete with references to similar experiences (Solomon et al., 1957) as are Charles Dickens’ observations on the hallucinations of prisoners in solitary confinement (Dickens, 1883), a phenomenon known as the “prisoner’s cinema”.

    • High altitude with monotonous environment has significant impact on mood and cognitive performance of acclimatized lowlanders: Possible role of altered serum BDNF and plasma homocysteine level

      2018, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, several studies have also reported mood behavioural changes in adverse environmental surroundings like HA (Heber, 1921; Bahrke and Hale, 1993; Elmore and Evans, 1983; Banderet and Burse, 1991; Bonnon et al., 1999; Bolmont et al., 2000; Bolmont et al., 2001). On the other hand, reports also showed that extended stay in HA, particularly with social deprivation, may lead to prefrontal lobe dysfunctions such as low resistance to stress and loss of inhibition (Arzy et al., 2005). Thus, social interaction is an important aspect of life to maintain normal health, mood function, behaviour and cognition (House et al., 1988; Cohen, 2004; Ditzen et al., 2008; Townsend and McWhirter, 2005; Cinini et al., 2014).

    • Brain mechanisms in religion and spirituality: An integrative predictive processing framework

      2017, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Importantly, the predictive processing framework also offers a powerful theoretical tool to understand the consequences of mystical and spiritual experiences at both a behavioral, brain and developmental level of investigation. Think for instance about how a mountaineer undergoes a process of spiritual transformation upon having a mystical experience at a mountain (Arzy et al., 2005). Initially, the mountaineer has a clearly defined representation of his/her bodily self (e.g., the body is constrained in space and time, the body constitutes our centre of conscious experience etc.).

    • Ganzfeld-induced hallucinatory experience, its phenomenology and cerebral electrophysiology

      2008, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Ganzfeld-like conditions may be met in exceptional natural environments, e.g., during high-altitude flights, exploratory journeys in mountain or desert landscapes, or practice of extreme sports. Individuals prone to hallucinatory experience in the ganzfeld may under such ‘favourable’ circumstances perceive unreal things and beings (cf. Brugger et al., 1999; Arzy et al., 2005). While in experimental situations the subjects are aware of the illusory character of their percepts, conditions for apprehension of percepts spontaneously occurring in the ‘natural ganzfeld’ may be different.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text